Powered by
Sponsored by

External influences affect children's development of self-control

How humans use a variety of external factors to organise their behaviour

handwriting-child-school-Indian-woman-helping-her-daughter-with-homework-shut

The ability to control one's own behaviour, known as executive function, might not exist all in your head. A new theory proposes that it develops with many influences from outside the mind.

Working memory, flexible thinking, time management, organisation and self-control are among the mental skills that constitute executive function. Having problems with these mental skills can make it difficult to manage daily life. 

The theory, detailed in Perspectives on Psychological Science, draws on dynamic systems theory which originated in mathematics and physics and has been used to describe complex organizing phenomena like cloud formation and flying patterns of birds. 

Now, a research team led by Washington State University human development assistant professor Sammy Perone is applying it to executive function, which has been shown to play a role in everything from children's readiness for school to their social relationships. Its development is also tied to long-term outcomes for adulthood.

"We propose that executive function is really about using cues from the environment to guide your behaviour," said Perone. "As humans, we use our experience and norms to decide what's the appropriate path to take, so to encourage executive function development, we want to help children build those connections between cues and appropriate behaviours."

In a classroom, these cues might include things such as decorations on the wall, verbal instructions or the way tables are set up. Eliminating environmental distractions may also help children control their behaviour like having sharpened pencils on hand or resolving a tottering desk chair. Besides, physical things normally thought of as peripheral, like whether a child has adequate sleep or enough to eat, also influence executive function, Perone said.

Previously, the dominant view held that executive function was three distinct neurocognitive processes: working memory, inhibitory control, which is the ability to stop yourself from doing something, and cognitive flexibility, which allows you to transition from one activity to the next. This perspective has been called into question, Perone said.

"If these different cognitive processes are what makes up executive function, you would think you could just train those processes, and then, you can then use them everywhere," he said. "Turns out, that doesn't work, and that's been shown over and over again. People think and behave in an environment, so we can't just train executive function by say doing computer exercises on working memory."

The new theory builds on the work of cognitive scientist Sabine Doebel who called for a redefinition of executive function in 2020 as the "development of skills in using the control in the service of behaviour." Perone and his co-authors from the University of Kansas and the University of Tennessee expand on that new definition by bringing in dynamic systems theory to help explain how humans use a variety of external factors to organise their behaviour.

"We need to think more about executive function as it operates—as goal-directed behaviour in the real world," Perone said. "When you take that perspective, all of the sudden, it becomes much more practical to childcare providers and parents, because that's where kids are thinking and behaving and developing."